Number Names and Words

Number Names

George Lakoff has pointed out that we do not normally distinguish numbers from what might be more properly be called number names. (Lakoff 1989)  The most common number naming systems adopt base-10 and use ten single-digit number names, for instance (0, 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 7, 8 and 9). They then form other multi-digit number names (21,  1342 etc) from these basic number names, or numerals, using a positional representation. With Arabic numerals this starts with the rightmost numeral being the quantity of units and the next leftward numeral being the quantity of tens and so on.

But many other bases are possible, the next most common probably being base-2 or binary, with just two basic number names (0 and 1). The difference between numbers and number names can therefore perhaps best be understood by realising that the number name ‘3’ in base-10 (3 × 100) represents the same quantity or numerosity as the number name ’11’ in base-2 ((1 × 21 ) + (1 × 20)). That is the base used, as well as the digits or glyphs adopted, can change the way any particular quantity, numerosity or number is represented.

Cardinal numbers measure the size of  collections or sets and therefore include the number zero needed to represent the size of an empty collection or set. In English cardinal numbers are nouns.

Ordinal numbers represent position or rank in a sequential, spatial or temporal lists or order and therefore do not include zero, there is no zeroth element in a sequential list. In English ordinal numbers are adjectives.

Number Words

Number names in this sense are different and distinct from number words, the verbal version of numbers, the way numbers are spoken or transliterated, (one, two, three etc.). see Five Finger Exercises

In English, verbal numbers are organised as a hybrid series of additions and multiplications summarised, for the cardinal Arabic number 350172, by the graph below where the plus signs indicate addition and the X signs multiplication.

/Users/grahamshawcross/Documents/blog_drafts/subitising/Subitisi

After Dehaene (1992) Varieties of Numerical Abilities Cognition, 44 1-42

So that ((((three is multiplied by a hundred) and added to fifty) which is then multiplied by a thousand) and added to ((one multiplied by a hundred) added to (seventy added to two)))

This system involves a combination of simple number words; one, two, three etc., some special multiplier words like hundred, thousand etc and the particularly English -ty words like sixty, seventy, eighty and ninety plus the slightly modified twenty, thirty, forty and fifty. And -teen words like thirteen, fourteen etc. plus the unique eleven and twelve.

With Arabic numerals the same cardinal number (350172) is represented positionally; starting with the rightmost numeral being the quantity of units (2) and the next leftward numeral being the quantity of tens (7) etc. Note that Arabic numbers are read, or more accurately generated, from right to left, perhaps betraying their origin.

Chinese number words follow a similar but somewhat simpler, more regular pattern.

Comparison of English and Chinese Number Words

ChineseNumberWordsSome of the extra complexity of English number words derive from spelling conventions rather than word sound, for instance eigh[]teen, fo[]rty and eigh[]ty. There is also some evidence of pronunciation slippage. Thus twelve and twenty to avoid the awkwardness of twoteen and twoty, thirteen and thirty to avoid threeteen and threety and fifteen and fifty to avoid fiveteen and fivety.

Ordinal Number Words

In English the initial verbal ordinal words are the unique firstsecond, and third, but typically ordinals have a th suffix added to the cardinal name for the number, so fourth, sixth, seventh, nineth, and tenth plus the slightly modified in spelling terms fif[]th, and eigh[]th. The multiple powers of ten have an ieth suffix replacing the y ending of the cardinal name, so twentieth from twenty, thirtieth from thirty, fortieth from forty etc. Again these are organised as a hybrid series of additions and multiplications.

/Users/grahamshawcross/Documents/blog_drafts/subitising/Subitisi

In English the Arabic version of ordinals borrow their suffices from the end of their verbal equivalents, so we have 1st (from first), 2nd (from second) and 3rd (from third), followed by 4th .. 20th then 21st, 22nd and 23rd etc.

The first three English ordinals have interestingly varied etymologies.  First derives from the Old English fyr(e)st and Old Norse fyrsthaving the sense of furthest forward, and the German Fürst, a prince, that is furthest forward in rank. Second derives from the Latin sequi followsecundus following and second via Old French into Middle English. Third derives from Old English thridda via English thrid which was the most common spelling until the 16th century.

A Latinate ordinal system is also used to represent importance and precedence,  primary, secondary, tertiary, quaternary etc. which are rarely used beyond the first four. So primarysecondary and tertiary education. This system is also used to indicate a sequence of  dependent effects, thus secondary picketing.

In technical and academic practice Greek ordinals are also used as prefixes proto-, deutero-, trite- and  tetarto-, thus proto-renaissance, protagonist and deuterium.

Fractions

When speaking of fractions a half is used for 1/2, a quarter for 1/4 and three quarters for 3/4 but a fourth is also used in music. Otherwise ordinals are used as in a third for 1/3, a fifth, a sixth etc. In the more general case a cardinal number is used for the numerator and an ordinal for the denominator, so 2/3 is two thirds and 19/32 is nineteen thirty seconds etc.

/Users/grahamshawcross/Documents/blog_drafts/children's counting

Transcoding

Literate English speakers have no problem reading, writing, comprehending or producing all these systems and transcoding between them even though there is evidence, through the study of patients with deficits in one or more of these capacities, of a neurological dissociation between the verbal and written systems (McCloskey 1992)

Bibliography

Dehaene, S. (1992) Varieties of Numerical Abilities Cognition, 44 1-42

Lakoff, G. (1987) Women, Fire, and Dangerous Things University of Chicago, Chicago Page 150

Lakoff, G. and Núñez, R. (2000) Where Mathematics Comes From Basic Books, New York

McCloskey, M. (1992) Cognitive mechanisms in numerical processing: Evidence from acquired dyscalculia, Cognition 44 107-157

About Graham Shawcross

Architect PhD Edinburgh University Interested in order, rhythm and pattern in Architectural Design
This entry was posted in Architecture, Classification, Enumeration, Logic and tagged , , , . Bookmark the permalink.

1 Response to Number Names and Words

  1. Marion Shawcross says:

    The pronunciation of the numeral ‘two’ is interesting in that the ‘w’ is silent whereas the ‘w’ in twelve, twenty, twice, etc is not. The word ‘two’ probably derived from the Old English ‘twa’ and the German ‘zwei’ and over time the ‘w’ sound was dropped. The old spelling remained, as a nod to the word’s history and a useful way to distinguish it from its homophones ‘too’ and ‘to’. To my way of thinking, sounding the ‘w’ in ‘two’ would be awkward, but missing it out in ‘twelve’ or ‘twenty’ would be awkward too. Perhaps this is just down to familiarity.

    There are a few other words with a connection to ‘two’ which include a ‘tw’, including twin, between, tweezer and our dear friends tweedledum and tweedledee.

    Like

Leave a comment